Abstract
This paper proposes
an environmental planning and management technique that reduces opposition to
and increases agreement with pro-environmental messages in the United States that
has been tested empirically. Widely held foundational American values of
significance to pro-environmental beliefs and actions that have been identified
and tested for significance through decades of empirical psychological research
are discussed. Material is presented on ways planners can use combined values
to frame pro-environmental measures that increase agreement and appeal to a broad
cross-section of citizens and stakeholders who may hold differing political
opinions and worldviews.
Introduction
Most North American planners
and managers who have organized and led public meetings on potentially
contentious environmental issues such as climate change, sustainability, land/forest/watershed
management, resource conservation, ecological planning, sprawl, low impact
development, or growth constraints/boundaries have run into disagreement and opposition
from various quarters (Illsleya and Richardson 2004). Although many times that controversy
stems from issue context, it also can result from pre-existing belief systems, such
as political or ideological orientation, held by citizens and stakeholders (Bain
et al. 2012; Feygina, Jost, and Goldsmith 2010; Gromet, Kunreuther, and Larrick
2013; Haidt and Graham 2007; Kahan 2013).
The use of various
techniques to resolve public opposition to or disputes over environmental issues
and reduce the possibility of struggle or adjudication has been studied in
several disciplines, including psychology, political science, economics, sociology,
planning, and law (Susskind, van der Wansem, and Ciccarelli. 2000). However, despite
the increasing use of consensus building and alternative dispute resolution
techniques (Innes and Booher 2004), the situation has not improved significantly
since Susskind and Weinstein (1980, 312) noted: “Disputes among groups with
conflicting values are epidemic.”
In the face of the
seemingly intractable nature of conflict, the question must be asked as to whether
a technique to maximize agreement and minimize opposition to pro-environmental
measures is available that has been tested empirically. In the planning
literature, Brody (2003a) observed the necessity of motivating communities to
protect ecosystem resources before they are lost or critically damaged but did
not identify measures to unify opponents that have been subject to empirical
testing. A number of planners interested in achieving broad-based support for
pro-environmental measures and have emphasized the importance of appealing to
shared environmental values (Kahn and Morris 2009; Lewis and Baldassare 2010).
However, to date no North American planning studies have grounded those
suggestions in empirical research that demonstrates the applicability of a
universal system of structured, shared values to pro-environmental measures. This
paper addresses that problem by focusing on the results of empirical research
in social psychology that demonstrates the efficacy of framing
pro-environmental messages to appeal to widely shared, foundational values,
which are defined formally as generalized internal criteria or standards that
transcend specific situations for “guiding action [and] for developing and
maintaining attitudes toward relevant objects and situations” (Rokeach 1968,
160) and informally as enduring standards or life goals that guide and motivate
human behavior.
The following elements
characterize the paper’s organizational structure. Problem-solving and
compromise-oriented strategies planners use to address public policy
decision-making and controversy are evaluated. Widely shared American values
and moral dimensions of direct significance to pro-environmental policies and
actions identified in empirical behavioral research are discussed. Pertinent empirical
literature is examined for evidence that identifies the roles widely shared
values/morals play in framing pro-environmental measures and for research that
addresses the close relationship of values to value-based environmental
concerns. A demonstration is provided showing that pro-environmental frames can
be organized around a single value/moral domain or multiple values and value-based
environmental concerns. A discussion is then presented on how environmental planners
can use information on widely shared values/moral domains combined with value-based
environmental concerns to frame pro-environmental measures that maximize agreement
and minimize disagreement over conflicting political or ideological positions while
appealing to a broad cross-section of conservatives, moderates, and liberals.
Various techniques and
models have been proposed to explain why people engage in behavior that
demonstrates levels of environmental awareness and action (Corbett 2006). Those
research frameworks include the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen &
Fishbein 1980; Fishbein & Ajzen 1975), model of responsible behavior
(Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera 1986-1987), model of human values (Schwartz 1994a),
deliberative and inclusionary processes (O’Riordan 2005), reasonable person
model (Kaplan 2000), community-based social marketing (McKenzie-Mohr 2000a,
2000b), social-ecological framework (Kurz 2002), and others. Kurz not only
presented ideas concerning the application of a social-ecological framework to
studying pro-environmental behavior[1]
(PEB) but also reviewed and discussed the limitations of several common
approaches to such research. For another, differently focused review of
selected frameworks for analyzing PEB, see Kollmuss and Agyeman (2002), who
also proposed a model that categorized a number of conflicting and competing
factors that shape environmental decisions. A vigorous and thought-provoking
critique of attitude-behavior-context models as applied to pro-environmental
actions and their effects on governance is provided by Shove (2010), who
criticizes what she characterizes as the limited vocabulary of the model that
“constrains and prevents policy imagination of the kind required” (2010, 1282)
to address society's relationship to climate change.
Many variables affect or
influence environmental behavior, including childhood experiences in nature,
images of environmental destruction, threat of natural disaster, emotions-affect
such as biophilia and topophilia, values, attitudes, environmental awareness,
sense of responsibility, injunctive and personal norms, etc. Values were chosen
as the key focus of this paper in terms of framing pro-environmental measures
for several reasons. First, values are important to environmental research
because their effects are empirically measurable (de Groot and Steg 2009; Karp
1996). Second, numerous empirical studies in psychology and sociology since the
1960s have identified widely shared values as criteria involved in guiding the
decisions and actions that constitute human behavior and in developing
attitudes toward environmental objects and issues (Hitlin and Piliavin 2004;
Rokeach 1973; Schwartz 1994a). Third, numerous social psychological studies have
determined that changing the personal values of individuals is difficult since
they are relatively stable and persist over time (Rokeach and Ball-Rokeach
1989; Schwartz 1992). And fourth, as a result of independent, empirical
analyses, psychologists have argued that values are an integral part of the framework
that connects values, beliefs, and norms to PEB and are more powerful in
explaining personal norms and intentions than worldviews (Steg et al. 2011; Stern
and Dietz 1994).
Values are significant
for their longevity, stability, and influence with respect to guiding
environmental attitudes and behavior (Stern 2000). Therefore, if planners/managers
can better understand the roles widely shared values and value-based environmental
concerns play in stimulating behavior, they may be more capable of using those
core beliefs to frame pro-environmental measures that appeal to those who hold
differing political opinions and worldviews. Although little environmental
planning/management research has been conducted into core values, an important
exception to that generalization can be found in the work of various U.S.
Forest Service scientists regarding what is known as values suitability
analysis (as reported in Brown and Reed 2000; Reed and Brown 2003). That
research was an effort to understand “the relationships between preferences and
attitudes toward forest management activities and forest values” (Brown and
Reed 2000, 240). However, no attempt was made to place “forest values” within
an overarching theoretical system that identified the universal content and
structure of human values and differentiated among various types of widely
shared values and their effects on environmental behavior.
It is critical to
emphasize that the widely shared values with which this paper is concerned are
the foundational beliefs that underlie the very motivations for human behavior.
Those values, as well as closely related value-based environmental concerns,
are not to be confused or conflated
with issue-based environmental concerns, principles, standards, or ethics such
as cherishing clean air and water, choosing green travel alternatives, supporting
the restoration of urban forests, or promoting ecologically sustainable
development and biodiversity (Gregory, McDaniels, and Fields 2001; Kahn and
Morris 2009; Merrick and Garcia 2004; Norton and Steinemann 2001). This paper
focuses on the core values and the related concerns that are responsible for
motivating people to treasure free-flowing streams, protect native flora and
fauna, reduce their carbon footprints, and plan more environmentally
responsible cities (Dietz, Fitzgerald, and Shwom 2005).
Techniques
to Resolve Controversy over Environmental Measures
The problem-solving and
compromise-oriented techniques planners use today to balance sensitive land-based
issues with politics at varying levels of governance have been influenced by behavioral
research in the fields of organizational psychology (Lewicki, Weiss, and Lewin 1992)
and negotiation/dispute resolution (Hensler 2003; Susskind 2009). Those engagement/dispute
resolution techniques include negotiation and mediation (Susskind, van der
Wansem, and Ciccarelli 2000), framing-reframing-reconceptualizing issues (Asah
et al. 2012; Kaufman and Gray 2003; Shmueli 2008), and resolving intractable conflicts
(Burgess and Burgess 2006; Campbell 2003; Lewicki, Gray, and Elliott 2003). Other
similar and at least partially related techniques for reducing or circumventing
land use/environmental conflicts include consensus building (Innes 1996; Innes
and Booher 2004), joint fact-finding (Karl, Susskind, and Wallace 2007), cooperative
discourse (Renn 1999), science impact coordination (Susskind and Karl 2009),
and a method known variously as communicative planning, collaborative planning,
shared decision making, or deliberative planning. That last technique, however
labeled, has numerous supporters (Forester 1999; Frame, Gunton, and Day 2004; Healey
1996, 2003; Innes 1995) as well as critics (Allmendinger and Tewdwr-Jones 2002).
Gregory, McDaniels, and
colleagues (Arvai, Gregory, and McDaniels 2001; Gregory 2000; McDaniels and
Gregory 2004) have articulated an approach to environmental decision-making in
a public policy context as an alternative to alternative dispute resolution and
consensus building that they contend “pose impediments to the creation of
insights for decision-makers and lead to the adoption of inferior policy
choices” (Gregory, McDaniels, and Fields 2001, 415). Their model uses a
structured approach of learning through adaptive management that was modified
from behavioral decision research and decision analysis combined with
value-focused thinking. According to the authors, that approach leads to
improved insight as well as more thoughtful, better informed, and higher
quality decisions by focusing simultaneously on deliberation and analysis.
However, one major difficulty
with the Habermasian and Giddensian approaches that are inherent in consensus
building, collaborative planning, and the structured learning approach is not
one of theory but human nature. Since the Habermasian and Giddensian approaches
to those and similar strategies are grounded in rationality (Booher and Innes
2002), they are vulnerable to criticism that a great many human actions are
less characterized by rational behavior than they are by uncertainty and non-rational[2]
behavior (Jensen 1994; Turner 1991). As Schon and Rein (1995) point out,
appeals to rationality can be ineffective in conflict situations since both
sides frequently use the very same fact-based evidence to support opposing contentions.
Thus, theories or models based on normative rationality can be criticized for failing
to account for observed behavior. Such criticism includes cognitive dissonance
(Festinger 1957), framing effects (Tversky and Kahneman 1981), system
justification (Feygina, Jost, and Goldsmith 2010), biased assimilation/attitude
polarization (Boysen and Vogel 2007), identity-protective cognition (Kahan et al.
2012), ideologically motivated cognition (Kahan 2013), confirmation bias (Taber
and Lodge 2006), belief congruence (Struch and Schwartz 1989), intuition (Dane
and Pratt 2007), social identity/party identification (Achen 2002), and party
polarization (McCright and Dunlap 2011) among numerous others. Many if not the
greater majority of psychologists believe that behaving non-rationally or
inconsistently is a universal human trait (Jensen 1994). Thus, techniques based
on rational models of behavior have been regarded by many as limited in
application.
The conflict-reducing
and decision-making techniques listed above have similar constraints in terms
of applicability for local jurisdictions as they are time consuming, expensive,
require highly trained individuals with special skills, and would likely entail
the involvement of outsiders—mediators, arbiters, negotiators, attorneys, and other
specialists—in the decision-making process, a condition many jurisdictions
regard unfavorably (Wondolleck and Yaffee 2000). And, as Innes (2004, 5)
pointed out, some of those techniques are “only appropriate in situations of
uncertainty and controversy where all stakeholders have incentives to come to
the table and mutual reciprocity in their interests.” Kolb and Rubin (1991),
Pruitt (1995), and Pruitt et al. (1993) also determined that when the central
part of a conflict involved moral or ethical dimensions, and by extension
values, agreements become difficult to achieve.
Since opposing
political/ideological views are typically grounded in strongly held principles,
using negotiation and dispute resolution techniques in environmental
disagreements driven by moral/value differences between conservatives and
liberals is problematic. Although many individuals may be willing to compromise
on disputed planning or policy-related issues, few would do so regarding deeply
held moral/ethical standards or values. Another critical factor is that
although dispute reduction-resolution and framing/reframing techniques have
proved beneficial in many diverse planning and policy-related situations,
typically they are initiated well after controversy has occurred and are not
concerned with maximizing agreement or minimizing opposition in the initial
stages of message framing, which is one of the primary interests of this paper.
Framing
of Environmental Issues in Planning Literature
A number of
planning-related papers that feature framing or reframing environmental issues
or values are identified in this section. The intent is to provide a snapshot
of the type of work on framing environmental issues in planning, not a formal
literature review. Academic papers that address the framing of those issues in
other disciplines, such as sociology or political science, are ignored unless
their focus is planning-related. A number of planners have used the concept of
framing to indicate a frame of reference, policy orientation, or varying
concepts of space and place (Cantrill and Senecah 2001; Faludi 2000; Healey
2004); those works are not included in this discussion.
As a topic in
urban-environmental planning research, framing has received only modest
attention. One of the first planners to recognize the power of geospatial
images to frame verbal statements of policy was Faludi (1996). That discussion
was continued by Ole Jensen and Richardson (2003) in an effort to broaden the
vocabulary of analysis from merely relying on narrative to include such graphic
representations as maps, images, and spatial metaphors that can be used to
frame geospatial identity, particularly in political struggles over policy
formation.
Malczewski et al. (1997)
developed a consensus-building model for framing group decision making that was
applied to environmental conflict over land resource allocation in the Cape
Region of Baja California Sur, Mexico. Their research evaluated a set of
feasible land use patterns on the basis of multiple, conflicting, non-commensurate
criteria provided by a group of stakeholders. Their model integrated the
Analytic Hierarchy Process, a technique for structuring the decision problem
and determining suitability for different land uses, and an integer
mathematical programming method that identified the land use pattern that
maximized consensus among interest groups.
Although not strictly a
work in planning, Dorcetta Taylor’s in-depth analysis of the application of a
powerful collective action frame (injustice) to effectively transform national
environmental discourse focused directly on the complex intertwined urban
issues of race, class, environment, and social justice (Taylor 2000). She
skillfully documented the use of several closely related master frames—civil
rights, segregation, institutionalized racism, and justice—that were reframed
and woven together to form an inseparable whole, the Environmental Justice
Paradigm widely used in urban sustainability planning (Campbell, S. 1996), that
was linked to concepts of environment and justice shared by the larger society,
and not only by minority Americans or socioeconomically disadvantaged whites.
Kaufman and Smith (1999)
analyzed issue and conflict frames used by participants in an
environmental/land use conflict in the Cleveland metropolitan area over the
proposed expansion of a landfill. They considered a range of roles practicing
planners might play, including supplying and interpreting information as
unbiased or uncommitted technical experts, actively advocating a specific
position, reframing issue messages, and providing a type of transformative
mediation.
Gregory, McDaniels, and
colleagues (Arvai, Gregory, and McDaniels 2001; Gregory 2000; Gregory,
McDaniels, and Fields 2001; McDaniels and Gregory 2004) have articulated an
approach to environmental decision-making in a public policy context as an
alternative to alternative dispute resolution and consensus building, which
they contended “pose impediments to the creation of insights for
decision-makers and lead to the adoption of inferior policy choices” (Gregory,
McDaniels, and Fields 2001, 415). Their model uses a structured approach
adapted from behavioral decision research and decision analysis combined with
value-focused thinking to lead to improved insight as well as more thoughtful,
better informed, and higher quality decisions by focusing simultaneously on
deliberation and analysis. They argued that their technique permits a better
understanding of participant preferences and enables participants and
decision-makers to consider and discuss a wider variety of decision-relevant
issues and address vital value trade-offs that may prove critical to success.
The environmental values that were investigated consisted of issue-based
concerns, such as water quality, jobs, consequences of flooding, and social
effects.
In a study of eight
intractable environmental/land use conflicts, Elliott, M., Gray, and Lewicki
(2002) determined that shifts in frames, as well as reframing, can help make
those conflicts less intractable. They found that conflicts may be more about
the loss of control or identity among participants in the process than about
specific issues and that stable frames persisting over time may promote
intractability. Elliott, M., and colleagues concluded that reframing combined
with tension and hostility reduction techniques and perspective-taking
processes can help establish common ground that can serve as the foundation for
agreement (for similar studies see Asah et al. 2012; Elliott, M. and Hanke
2002; Lewicki, Gray, and Elliott 2003; Shmueli, Elliott, and Kaufman 2006;
Shmueli 2008). A closely related effort (Elliott, M. and Kaufman 2003)
documented the use of shared discursive and interpretive frames for
understanding and acting on environmental issues and civic discourse/problem solving
to build civic capacity to resolve significant environmental problems despite
the existence of extensive prior community conflict (for additional related
information see Burgess and Burgess 1996, 2006; Elliott, M. 2003; Kaufman,
Gardner, Burgess 2003).
Changes in urban
governance in the last several decades through such innovations as citizen
involvement in decision-making, collaborative visioning, and public-private
partnerships led McCann (2003) to investigate the role discursive framing plays
in issues that involve the complex interaction of geospatial and temporal
scales. He argued that as scalar priorities are reorganized, from
jurisdiction-wide scale to the smaller neighborhood scale of collaborative and
smart growth planning, the existing nested political hierarchy would be
subjected to stress and may experience organizational problems that are very
difficult to overcome.
The Cities for Climate
Protection Campaign (CCPC) was critically evaluated by Lindseth (2004) in terms
of its failure to use issue framing to establish climate change in local
political jurisdictions as an overarching and principal responsibility rather
than simply as a one of a number of political agendas vying for attention.
Lindseth argued that more appropriately crafted frames would allow the CCPC to
focus not only on local climate change mitigation and adaptation techniques but
also on forming policies that reject unsustainable development.
Struggles involving a
federal housing program in New Orleans and principles of New Urbanism were
investigated in terms of the framing tactics used by opposing groups (Elliott,
J., Gotham, and Mulligan 2004). Groups on opposite sides of that conflict
attempted to legitimize their positions and delegitimize their opponents using
highly fluid tactics that involved framing, reframing, and counter-framing. The
authors concluded that struggles over urban space have the potential of
developing into framing contests in which opposing parties attempt to win by
defining and interpreting the key issues and identifying themselves as the
“good guys” and their opponents as the “bad guys” in terms of whose proposals
were more in tune with the principles of New Urbanism and inner city
neighborhood design.
Merrick and Garcia
(2004) applied a value-focused thinking process to making decisions about how
to improve the current condition of an urban watershed in the Richmond,
Virginia, area. They focused on the issue-based values and preferences (e.g.
restoring the natural stream channel and improving water quality) of a panel of
16 decision makers rather than on a limited set of watershed improvement
alternatives. A model for measuring the quality of the watershed was
constructed from the decision makers’ values and preferences and was compared
to the results for a hypothetical watershed to reveal gaps or shortcomings. The
issue-based values and preferences investigated were focused on the conditions
of a specific watershed and not on widely shared core values (see also Merrick
et al. 2005).
Analysis of a large-scale
urban development project in Europe (Gualini and Majoor 2007) sought to
determine how discursive framing on the urban quality of places could affect
collective choices and possibly lead to integrating public and private
interests (see also Majoor 2011; Salet 2008).
In a random survey of
suburban residents, Goetz (2008) investigated whether the words planners use to
frame potentially controversial urban issues and policies like affordable
housing affected public support. He determined the difference was significant
among white, non-Hispanic respondents but insignificant among non-White
respondents. Goetz concluded that, given appropriate circumstances, planners
might be able to expand policy options previously thought infeasible through
the use of carefully articulated issue frames.
In an empirical analysis
of associations between community environmentalism and green travel behavior,
Kahn and Morris (2009) discussed green beliefs/values as causative agents of
what the authors label “voluntary restraint” regarding green travel behavior.
However, the authors did not differentiate among environmental
actions/behaviors that are motivated by widely shared core values, value-based
environmental concerns, or issue-based environmental concerns, making it
difficult if not impossible to determine the specific cause(s) of the voluntary
restraint behaviors that were investigated.
Although Kennedy et al.
(2009) are not planners, their research on understanding the environmental
values/behavior gap was not published in a planning journal, and the emphasis
of their paper was on individual rather than collective behavior, their focus
on core environmental values and beliefs makes their research of interest to
this discussion. Kennedy and colleagues used social psychological research by
Schwartz (1994a, 1994b) and Stern and colleagues (Stern 2000; Stern and Dietz
1994, Stern et al. 1995; Stern et al. 1999) into the nature of values critical
to pro-environmental behavior to help them understand why Canadians are
characterized by a gap between self-identified pro-environmental beliefs and
behavior. They concluded that the lack of appropriate knowledge available to
individuals formed a significant hurdle to their practice of environmentally
supportive behavior and that well-timed and positioned information can be a
powerful behavioral incentive.
A study of complex
public attitudes regarding urban development by Lewis and Baldassare (2010)
found that although demographic attributes were strongly associated with
various views concerning tradeoffs, only conservative political ideology was
consistently associated with opposition to compact development. They concluded
that advocates of compact development might consider framing that development
to appeal more to political conservatives, which could be interpreted as a call
to focus on shared values. However, the implications of that finding or
techniques with which to implement their conclusion were not identified or
explored in their study.
In several discussions
of ecological planning, Ernst (2012) and Ernst and Weitz (2013) commented that
appealing to shared environmental values constituted a primary means of
generating public support in terms of social capacity and will for
pro-environmental measures. However, that suggestion focused on issue-based
environmental beliefs such as promoting biodiversity or protecting sensitive
habitat and was neither developed in terms of procedural detail nor grounded in
empirical research that demonstrated such an approach was feasible.
McEvoy, Fünfgeld, and
Bosomworth (2013) briefly examined four conceptual issue frames in the context
of climate change adaptation in Australia: hazards, risk management,
vulnerability, and resilience. They argued that the election of various issue
frames can lead to different types of climate change assessments, especially in
light of political/ideological factors and organizational preferences held by
stakeholders and policy-makers. Among their main conclusions was that issue
frames that emphasize resilience and strengthening local communities are
increasingly preferred by the public over frames that emphasize risk and
vulnerability.
Most articles on framing
or reframing environmental measures published in planning journals discuss
issue frames, conflict frames, or discursive frames and have not addressed the
concept of appealing to the widely shared values that serve as guiding
principles in a person’s life (see Rokeach 1973). Although a number of papers
were concerned with shared principles regarding environmental issue-based
topics (Arvai, Gregory, and McDaniels 2001; Ernst 2012; Ernst and Weitz 2013;
Gregory, McDaniels, and Fields 2001; Kahn and Morris 2009; Lewis and Baldassare
2010; and Merrick and Garcia 2004), they did not explore the use of widely
shared values in framing environmental measures. The only exception was Kennedy
et al. (2009) and their research was on individual behavior and not on
planning-related issues. The challenge, then, is to demonstrate that widely
shared American values can be linked to environmental behaviors of interest to
planners through a targeted message framing process that results in maximizing
agreement, mitigating constraints, and devising strategies that appeal to a
broad base of citizens and stakeholders regardless of political-ideological
orientation.
Widely Shared American Values and Pro-Environmental
Behavior
Empirical psychological research into methods for organizing values began
in the late 1960s and continues (Rokeach 1968 and 1971; Schwartz and Bilsky
1987; Triandis 1996; Weber and Stern 2011). As part of that empirical effort, Dunlap,
Grieneeks, and Rokeach (1983) argued that pro-environmental concerns and
behavior are rooted in human values. Somewhat later, Schwartz (1992, 1994)
created a widely accepted empirical model that classified values as falling
along two complex continua, from self-enhancement to self-transcendence and
from openness to change to conservatism-tradition. Schwartz’s research found that
Americans as a group score the highest on the self-interest dimension and relatively
low on self-transcendence (Poortinga, Steg, and Vlek 2004). He also determined
that Americans score high on openness to change (Markowitz et al. 2012) and low
on conservative values (Schultz and Zelezny 2003). Another key finding was that
scoring high on self-interest values did not correlate with scoring low on
self-enhancement. That determination is significant to this paper and to environmental
planners in that values held by people are differentiated, complex,
multi-layered, and not mutually exclusive. Having high self-interest values does
not indicate that all Americans endorse those values to the exclusion of others
or that aspects of both self-transcendence and self-interest cannot be held
simultaneously by the same individual at varying levels of intensity (Schultz
and Zelezny 2003). Thus,
pro-environmental actions not only can arise from multiple, conflicting values
but also from multiple, conflicting motivations and intentions (Brandon and
Lewis, 1999; Lindenberg and Steg 2007).
Research into PEB[3] by
De Young (1985, 1996) focused on intrinsic motivation or personal satisfaction,
a component of the self-interest value domain that is derived from
participation in pro-environmental activities. De Young’s investigation of the
structure of intrinsic satisfaction with respect to PEB found that it is a
complex, multi-dimensional variable, with four discernible aspects that he tested
in nine separate empirical studies over the course of a decade: satisfaction
from striving for personal competence; satisfaction from frugal, thoughtful
consumption; satisfaction from participating in community activities; and
satisfaction from comfort/convenience. Of the four aspects of intrinsic satisfaction,
De Young (2000, 522) identified the human desire for competence as a primary
source of behavioral motivation and argued that it “may readily explain the
conditions under which people will consider adopting ERB [Environmentally
Responsible Behavior].” De Young (1996, 391) argued “that competence motivation
is integral with human well-being” and also found that since behavior
associated with competence consists of highly focused activities it is
intrinsically reinforcing, meaning worth doing in its own right for its
inherent personal satisfactions. The blueprint De Young presented for framing
PEB as an exercise in competence included providing appropriate context
complete with readily available procedural information[4]
that allows and encourages actions to be taken in a supportive milieu.
Public involvement, the participation category identified by De Young
(1985) as generating intrinsic satisfaction, is one of the primary techniques
used to include the citizenry in meaningful contributions to the planning
process and to local governance (Brody 2003b; Brody, Godschalk, and Burby 2003;
Hawkins and Wang 2012; Susskind and Cruikshank 2006). De Young (2000) presented
participation as a value dimension that included a broad range of aspects,
including enjoyment-satisfaction derived from working with others to achieve
common goals and the willingness to spend time and effort to share skills and
knowledge. Kaplan (2000) regarded appropriately structured citizen involvement
that included participant-based problem-solving as a powerful counter to
feelings of helplessness and also regarded citizen participation as
“inextricably linked” to the framing of pro-environmental measures.
Consequently, focusing value-based frames on citizens and stakeholders working
together to solve environmental challenges (the drive for competence combined
with love of community and openness to change) in a collaborative,
participatory context is highly likely to generate broad-based support.
Research on the Self-Determination Theory (Deci and Ryan 2000; Ryan
and Deci 2000) indicated that the satisfaction of psychological needs for
competence and autonomy combined with “social contexts that support
satisfaction of those needs, promote the internalization of autonomous or
functional forms of regulation and well-being” (quoted in Pelletier and Sharp
2008, 211). The contention is that contexts that are autonomy-supportive
(encouraging people to make informed, un-coerced decisions) and informational,
which are characteristics of well-structured citizen participation efforts, are
likely to enable self-determined motivation and optimal functioning, elements
that are necessary for effective PEB.
In an effort to determine the basis for environmental behavior, Paul
Stern and various colleagues (Stern and Dietz 1994; Stern, Dietz, and Kalof
1993; Stern et al. 1995) proposed that attitudes of environmental concern (see Alibeli
and White 2011; Dunlap and Jones 2002) are rooted in a person’s value system
and that individuals could express the same level of general concern, for
example about the issue of water pollution, for the following fundamentally
different reasons. Polluted water may be dangerous to my health. Polluted water
may be dangerous to the health of children. Polluted water may damage
ecosystems. Stern and Dietz (1994) labeled those value-based environmental
concerns egoistic, social-altruistic, and biospheric. Egoistic concerns are
defined as self-interest based on an individual valuing self above others and
the environment and calculating benefits that would accrue personally before
making a decision. Social-altruistic concerns focus on others, such as family,
children, community, strangers, and humanity even when costs to self are
incurred (Batson 1994). Those individuals scoring high on biospheric concerns
are likely to decide whether to engage in pro-environmental acts by considering
apparent costs and benefits to something that might be as loosely-defined as
“nature” or as specific as an individual ecological niche (de Groot and Steg
2007). The research of Stern and others into the different value foundations of
environmental concern was further developed as the Value-Belief-Norm Theory,
which proposed that pro-environmental concerns and behaviors are likely to be
clustered around common value themes (Stern and Dietz 1994; Stern et al. 1999).
That research is significant as it demonstrates that egoistic, altruistic, and
biospheric concerns are related to pro-environmental intentions and behaviors
through a structured system of values, beliefs, and pro-environmental personal
norms.[5]
Since mere involvement in PEB does not distinguish among people who
exhibit value-based concerns regarding environmental problems (de Groot and
Steg 2007), additional research concentrated on explicating the value-based
dimensions and motivations of PEB. Cross-cultural survey data on the three
value-based environmental concerns and PEB revealed that social-altruistic dimensions
are the most highly rated (Schultz and Zelezny 2003). A second significant research
finding was that self-transcendent values positively correlate with biospheric
concerns while self-interest values relate negatively to biospheric concerns
(de Groot and Steg 2009). Despite those determinations, many social scientists
argued that, through application of appropriate framing techniques, self-interest
values are likely to lead to egoistic concerns for the environment and PEB (De
Young 2000). Empirical research by Bator and Cialdini (2000) and Cialdini
(2003) strengthened that position when they found that framing PEB public
service announcements in terms of self-interest values resulted in higher
positive responses than if the same measures were composed in
altruistic-biospheric value dimensions.
Research beginning in the 1980s determined that self-interest values (including
the closely related egoistic concerns) can work together with
altruistic-biospheric orientations to promote PEB (see De Young 2000; Stern et
al. 1995) and that self-interest values are consistent with altruistic motives
(Jensen 1994). In studies that examined variables leading to longer participation
in volunteering, Snyder and colleagues (Clary and Snyder 1999; Omoto and Snyder
1995; Snyder and Omoto 1991) reported that people who scored higher in terms of
self-oriented motives tend to volunteer for longer periods than people who hold
more social-altruistic values. Those findings are strong indications that
distinguishing among value-based environmental concerns is both relevant and significant
(Steg, Dreijerink, and Abrahamse 2005, 424) when planners frame
pro-environmental measures.
Appealing to altruism exclusively as a principal motivator for PEB has
a number of drawbacks, chief of which is it of necessity requires not only
self-sacrifice but also acting against self-interest (Kaplan 2000). Since
Americans as a group score high on self-interest and low on
altruism/self-transcendence, the difficulty of basing pro-environmental appeals
exclusively on altruistic or altruistic-biospheric concerns should be obvious. In
terms of the environment, altruism is also associated with giving up modern creature
comforts in exchange for an austere, even bleak, future (De Young 1990-1991),
characteristics unlikely to attract support from most Americans. A solution to
the uncertainty as to favoring altruistic or biospheric concerns regarding pro-environmental
measures was proposed by de Groot and Steg (2009, 5): “Egoistic values should always [emphasis in the original] be
linked to altruistic and biospheric values because it is ultimately the
altruistic and biospheric values that need to be salient to reach stable
pro-environmental behavior.” They contend that a strategy focused on making self-interest/egoistic
values compatible with altruistic-biospheric concerns has the best chance of
promoting stable PEB, which is a combined value-concern approach that has broad-based
appeal. The logic of that approach was indicated by Mansbridge (1990, 133), who
pointed out that altruistic concerns must coexist with self-interest within the
same individual “to prevent the extinction of either the altruistic motivation
or the altruist.”
In the 2000s, several social psychologists initiated a series of
empirical analyses to determine whether American concerns about the environment
had moral foundations (Haidt 2008). They found that environmental values are
directly related to moral dimensions and motivations and are also highly
polarized with respect to political ideology (Feinberg and Willer 2011 and 2013;
Graham, Haidt, and Nosek 2009; Graham et al. 2011). That empirical research
identified five moral foundations that appeal in varying degrees to both
liberals and conservatives: care/harm, fairness/cheating, loyalty/betrayal,
authority/subversion, and sanctity/degradation (Graham et al. 2013). Surveys
conducted by the moral foundation theorists demonstrated that conservatives value
all five foundations fairly evenly but liberals value the first two higher than
the others (Graham, Haidt, and Nosek 2009; McAdams et al. 2008). That research
also found that liberals and conservatives are often at cross purposes because
their values and judgments regarding environmental issues are based on
differing configurations of the five moral foundations. The authors determined
that both groups undervalue the validity of the moral systems held by the
other, though that condition is more characteristic of liberals than
conservatives.
Recent research found that the results of five empirical studies
conducted into the moral dimensions of environmental issues “suggest that
political polarization around environmental issues is not inevitable but can be
reduced by crafting proenvironmental arguments that resonate with the values of
American conservatives” (Feinberg and Willer 2013, 61). That research
determined that environmental discussions in the U.S. are based largely on
values related to harm and care, moral/ethical concerns more deeply held by
progressives than by conservatives and “that reframing proenvironmental
rhetoric in terms of purity, a moral value resonating primarily among
conservatives, largely eliminated the difference between liberals’ and
conservatives’ environmental attitudes” (Feinberg and Willer 2013, 56). They
concluded that political polarization around environmental issues is avoidable and
can be reduced by using pro-environmental frames that resonate with the values
of American conservatives, thus closing the gap between liberals and
conservatives in environmental concern and behavior, a contention that echoes
research on shared values and value-based concerns (De Young 1985, Kaplan 2000;
Schultz 2002; Schwartz 1994a).
Constructing
Pro-Environmental Frames to Generate Broad-Based Support
Many environmental planners work in local and
regional jurisdictions populated by citizens and stakeholders who hold ideological
positions ranging across the board in a complex potpourri of worldviews. To
function well, those planners must be able to present pro-environmental proposals
in a manner that enables their passage despite the mix of worldviews held by
people in the jurisdiction.
The question that must be addressed is how environmental
planners, the greater majority of whom may not be familiar with psychological research
into values, are going to be able to construct appropriate pro-environmental frames
that will elicit broad-spectrum support from the public based on the
information presented above. In recognition of those challenges, the materials
on environmental values, value-based concerns, and moral domains that are provided
in this section and in Table 1 constitute both practical guidelines and tools environmental
planners can use to construct frames that link PEB to people’s basic values and
their everyday lives.
The best way to minimize
opposition to environmental policy measures is not to present overwhelming scientific
facts or evidence but to frame the message in values/morals shared by the
parties involved and abandon the use of issue or collective action frames as
the principal method of communicating with the public. The
goals of minimizing opposition and generating broad-based support for
pro-environmental measures can be accomplished by applying the findings of
social science research to the
framing process, especially those that identify using self-interest/egoistic
values in combination with altruistic and biospheric concerns. Variations
of values, moral considerations, and value-based environmental concerns that
can be used to frame pro-environmental measures are provided in outline form in
Table 1 so planners can familiarize themselves with the concepts shorn of
embellishment. Those elements have been collected from empirical social science
research studies discussed herein.
Table 1: Value-Based Concerns/Morals for
Framing Pro-Environmental Messages
|
|
Concerns
|
Specific Sub-Concerns
|
Egoistic
|
Personal
love of and pride in country/culture/community.
|
Concern
for one’s own financial betterment.
|
|
Desire
for self to have a better quality of life.
|
|
Personal
well-being/health.
|
|
Preserving
natural places makes me feel spiritually renewed.
|
|
Experiencing
natural places helps me learn about the environment
|
|
Personal
enjoyment/satisfaction gained through competence, participation in community,
frugality, comfort/convenience.
|
|
Living
responsibility by following rules, norms, or procedures.
|
|
Altruistic
|
A world
free of war and environmental conflicts brings peace to all.
|
Parks
and natural areas are for everyone to enjoy.
|
|
Working towards
environmental justice helps the poor and disadvantaged.
|
|
Save our
parks, natural areas, and the planet for future generations.
|
|
Planting
native species in my yard will benefit all our neighbors.
|
|
Beautifying
our community benefits all residents and visitors.
|
|
Improve
the natural experience for all by removing exotic species.
|
|
Living a
simpler life and lowering our carbon footprint helps poor people in our local
community and all over the world.
|
|
Biospheric
|
Respect
the Earth.
|
Live in
harmony with nature.
|
|
Nature
should be appreciated for its own sake.
|
|
Humans
are an intrinsic part of nature.
|
|
All
parts of the natural world are interconnected.
|
|
Conserve/restore
the natural world.
|
|
Protect
endangered species
|
|
Increasing
biodiversity will secure a sustainable future for all.
|
|
Controlling
waste and pollution will benefit nature.
|
|
Using
renewable resources is better for the Earth.
|
|
Protect
habitats used by traditional cultures.
|
|
Promote
organic farming.
|
|
Cutting
back on our consumer lifestyle will help the Earth recover.
|
|
Moral
Domains
|
Fairness/reciprocity:
We owe it to future generations to protect the environment.
|
Harm/care:
The best way to care for the Earth is to increase biodiversity and protect
native species.
|
|
In-group/loyalty:
Working together we can reduce CO2 emissions while growing the economy and
protecting our way of life.
|
|
Authority/respect:
Providing fresh air and clean water is a way to show our respect for the
Earth.
|
|
Purity/sanctity/stewardship:
We can cleanse the Earth by removing harmful pollutants and hazardous wastes.
|
In the approach to constructing
pro-environmental frames to generate broad-based support proposed herein, measures
can be framed using a single value/moral domain or combinations of
values/morals and value-based concerns. Based on empirical research in social
psychology, it is the author’s contention that combining self-interest and
openness to change with various value-based environmental concerns
(egoistic-biospheric-altruistic) and moral domains is the most effective
approach in terms of creating the broadest-based support for environmental measures.
However, since framing measures in terms of shared values is not a technique
with which many environmental planners have first-hand experience, the
sub-sections below provide examples of message frames created by national
environmental organizations as well as examples generated by the author. Examples
are provided for individual environmental concerns/moral domains and for
combinations of different values/concerns/morals to demonstrate the importance
of using multiple orientations in creating pro-environmental messages. In each
of the examples provided, value frames take precedence over issue frames, which
play secondary roles if they appear at all. Note that the frames below are
positive and do not project negative images of the environment that could make
people uncomfortable,
invoke rejection responses, or send a muddled message that environmentally
irresponsible behavior is widespread and therefore acceptable even if not
socially approved (Cialdini 2003).
Individual Value/Concern Frames
Examples of frames using each of the three individual
value-based concerns—egoistic, altruistic, and biospheric—and moral domains are
provided below. Three of the four examples start with slogans or mission
statements from nationally-known environmental organizations that are followed
by author-generated frames.
Egoistic Frames
A. “Wilderness belongs to you. It’s a place to enjoy.”[6]
The Wilderness Society
B. “Explore, enjoy and protect the planet.”[7]
Sierra Club
C. I walk or bike to work almost every day because I want the air in my
city to be as clean as possible.
D. I am relaxed and at peace when I walk in wooded areas around the city
because I feel I can make a difference by being a just steward of the Earth.
E. If you love fishing in clean, free-flowing streams, the County’s
Annual Stream Improvement Project is a perfect opportunity to make a difference.
The emphasis on self-enhancing concerns in the
professionally crafted slogans issued by national environmental organizations
is echoed in the next three frames. Note that the self-enhancing aspect of all
five measures is not off-putting or jarring as it might have been if the appeal
had been to interests that were exclusively self-centered, which points to the critical
difference between self-enhancing messages and selfishness. Examples C and E
also contain an implied openness to change dimension.
Altruistic Frames
A. “To discuss and explore the linkages between environmental quality and
social justice, and to promote dialogue, increased understanding and
appropriate action.”[8]
Sierra Club
B. “Inspiring Americans to protect wildlife for our children’s future.”[9]
National Wildlife Federation
C. If developed countries, like the U.S. and England, and developing
nations, like India and China, adopt low-carbon lifestyles, people all over the
world will benefit.
D. Protect the Earth for future generations by reducing energy use today.
E. Adopt low-carbon lifestyles that include walking or biking to work and
running errands to ensure everyone has clean air to breathe and a better
quality of life.
The emphasis on altruistic concerns in the
professionally crafted slogans issued by the two national environmental
organizations is echoed in the next three frames in such phrases as “people all
over the world,” “future generations,” and “everyone has clear air to breathe.”
Note that the author-generated examples also imply openness to change.
Biospheric Frames
A. “Building a future in which people live in harmony with nature.”[10]
World Wildlife Fund
B. “We work to preserve the natural systems on which all life depends,
focusing on the most critical environmental problems.”[11]
Environmental Defense Fund
C. Celebrate Earth Day by planting native vegetation in your backyard.
D. Love and respect your Mother Earth.
E. Help your city adopt urban-ecological planning practices that promote
biodiversity and protect sensitive environments.
The emphasis on biospheric concerns in the professionally
crafted slogans issued by the World Wildlife Fund and the Environmental Defense
Fund is echoed in the next three author-generated frames that use such phrases
as “planting native vegetation,” “your Mother Earth,” and “promote biodiversity.”
Four of the five frames are positive and do not project negative images of the
environment. Although the second example frame, that of the Environmental
Defense Fund, mentions environmental problems, it does so only in general terms
and provides no details that would enhance the formation of negative images or
send mixed measures that could be misinterpreted. As in the subsection above,
the author-generated examples also imply openness to change.
Moral Domain Frames
A. Working with your family, friends, neighbors, and people in the
community can result in solving many local environmental problems.
Moral
domains appealed to:
in-group
loyalty; love of community
B. Breathing foul air and drinking contaminated water is disgusting; as
stewards of the Earth we must clean up our environment.
Moral
domains appealed to: purity/sanctity/stewardship
Because appealing to moral domains held by
conservatives is a relatively new approach, no examples from national
environmental organizations have been found. Therefore, all the examples
provided above are author-generated. Note that since two moral domains,
care/harm and fairness/reciprocity, appeal largely to liberals and are used
frequently in framing environmental messages and the third, loyalty/betrayal,
has minimal environmental involvement they are not included above.
Combined Frames
The various value orientations and value-based
concerns combined in the frames below are listed after each individual frame.
Since the self-interest/egoistic orientation has multiple variations, those dimensions
are listed separately. As in the subsection above, the first framing examples
are those created by national environmental organizations and the ones that
follow are author-generated.
A. “To conserve and restore natural ecosystems, focusing on birds, other
wildlife, and their habitats for the benefit of humanity and the Earth’s
biological diversity.”[12]
National Audubon Society
Value-based concerns appealed to: biospheric, altruistic.
B. “We protect wildlife because they inspire us.”[13]
World Wildlife Fund
Value-based concerns appealed to: biospheric, egoistic.
C. Riding my bike to work and to shop instead of driving is better for my
wallet, my personal health, the community I live in, future generations, and
for the Earth; plus I’ve lost thirty pounds since I started riding.
Values/concerns appealed to: self-interest/egoistic (competence,
frugality, love of community, personal gain, personal wellbeing), altruistic, biospheric;
the openness to change value is also implied.
D. Respecting the Earth in every neighborhood in our city is a key for
you, your family, and your friends/neighbors being able to work together to
build good, healthy, productive lives.
Values/concerns appealed to: biospheric, altruistic, self-interest/egoistic
(love of family/community, participation, competence, personal wellbeing,
personal gain); the openness to change value is also implied.
E. Working together to restore the natural integrity of our community’s
ecosystems and habitats benefits the future for us and our families, our
quality of life, and the Earth itself.
Values/concerns appealed to: self-interest/egoistic (competence,
participation-problem solving, love of family/community), biospheric, and
altruistic; the openness to change value is also implied. Moral domains
appealed to: purity/sanctity, environmental stewardship, love of
family/community.
With the exception of the slogans or mission
statements of the national environmental organizations, all the frames above
combine egoistic, altruistic, and biospheric concerns and also appeal to
conservative moral dimensions through the love of family and community. In
addition, with the exception of those created by environmental organizations, the
frames include at least three types of self-interest/egoistic values and most
have more. That focus
is intentional and follows numerous research findings regarding self-interest
values/concerns and PEB (de Groot and Steg 2009; De Young 2000; Kaplan 2000; Schultz
2002; Schwartz 1994a). All the
combined value/concern frames except Audubon’s are self-oriented and
community-oriented (Loroz 2007) and most indirectly appeal to being open to
change in that the sought after action is not currently in effect. Pro-environmental
frames that appeal to combined values/concerns may differ somewhat in tone and focus
from traditional planning frames, but the underlying meanings are not greatly
different.
Discussion
Solely relying on facts to persuade is fraught
with difficulty (Schon and Rein 1995), especially since opposing parties
frequently use the same facts to argue different sides of the same issue
(Elliott, Gotham, and Mulligan 2004). Appealing to objective, scientific
evidence to win an argument can run afoul of deeply held beliefs and ideologies
(Haidt, Graham, and Joseph 2009; Kahan 2013). But framing pro-environmental messages
with values and value-based concerns already shared by most Americans presents environmental
planners and managers with opportunities to find common ground by avoiding politically
and ideologically charged positions and maximizing agreement.
Many pro-environmental messages use altruistic
and biospheric frames (e.g. caring for the environment and fairness/justice for
future generations) that appeal largely to liberals, some moderates, and relatively
few conservatives (De Young 2000; Schultz and Zelezny 2003). Creating frames
with broad-spectrum appeal centered on self-interest/egoistic values requires
changing that status quo and weaving together several shared value/moral threads.
Although those value/moral positions may have separate origins, when combined
and applied to message frames they increase the effectiveness of the message
and motivate individuals to adopt pro-environmental measures despite differences in political
beliefs (Feinberg and Willer 2013). Depending on issue
context, those shared self-interest/egoistic domains should be combined with value-based
altruistic and biospheric concerns to create environmental messages that will
be more accepted and supported across the political/ideological spectrum than measures
that only appeal to those who have biospheric or altruistic orientations (de
Groot and Steg 2009).
Planners and managers who are committed to
finding the most appropriate value frames for pro-environmental measures should
be aware of a conclusion on human behavior reached by social psychologist
Daniel Batson through empirical research: “We may be social in thought and
action, but in motivation we are capable of caring only for ourselves” (cited
in De Young 1996, 394). A number of empirical studies demonstrate that framing
pro-environmental measures to appeal to self-interest/egoistic value
orientations, especially love of community/culture, community/civic benefit, family
safety/security, and the measures of intrinsic satisfaction—competence,
participation, frugality, and comfort/convenience—is likely to increase the
acceptance of environmental measures and achieve broad-based support (Bain et
al. 2012; De Young 1996; Kaplan 2000; Kennedy et al. 2009). Those value/moral dimensions
can easily be woven together by environmental planners into a participation-oriented,
problem-solving fabric that would bypass opposition and appeal to a broad
audience. As social psychologists contend, PEB is not determined by a single
variable or motivation but is multiply determined (Allen and Ferrand 1999; Geller
1995; Moisander 2007) and is associated with multiply desirable choices (Kaplan
2000). Consequently, the
primary task for environmental planners desiring to create pro-environmental measures
that maximize agreement is to frame those measures with several value/moral orientations
that are congruous with the self-interest value most Americans hold dear and that
are appropriate to the context of the issue. Accomplishing that goal requires
identifying value/moral domains that engage widely shared motivations and
combining those values with value-based biospheric-altruistic concerns and
openness to change. As Goetz (2008) has demonstrated, as planners/managers our
choice of words and frames make a difference.
Creating
pro-environmental measures that resonate with moral values held by
conservatives requires understanding how those moral dimensions relate to
self-interest and egoistic values and also to altruistic-biospheric concerns (Feinberg
and Willer 2013; Graham et al. 2011). For example, the role of purity and
sanctity with respect to protecting streams and natural landscapes from
pollution and wastes that can contaminate people should be evident. That moral
protection of self and others and nature fits well with the mainstream American
values discussed herein. In-group loyalty as a moral foundation is strikingly
similar to self-interest values (love of community and the closely related civic
pride). Therefore, careful combination of the moral foundations that appeal to
conservatives with self-interest/egoistic values and openness to change has the
potential of reducing partisan polarization on pro-environmental issues and
increasing broad-based support.
The combination of
specific values/morals will vary with the nature of the environmental policy/program
and especially with context and local knowledge and capabilities, indicating
that for value frames to be successful they must also address local conditions
and trends. The goal for planners/managers is to craft pro-environmental measures
that are grounded in local context and will appeal to a diversity of potential
respondents, including those for whom self-interest/egoistic concerns dominate
and those for whom biospheric-altruistic orientations are predominant. Thus,
the primary focus for planners/managers in creating pro-environmental messages should
not be on the issue itself but on linking widely shared environmental values/concerns/morals
to desired actions and outcomes.
Concluding
Remarks
Many environmental planners/managers instinctively
try to frame measures to find consensus or a common dominator in their
audiences without being aware of the logic behind those efforts. This paper
presents an approach to that process that is based on more than four decades of
empirical research in social psychology into how widely shared values are
related to environmental behavior. A significant quotation from empirical
research conducted into the moral dimensions of environmental issues that was
cited above bears repeating: “. . . political polarization around environmental
issues is not inevitable but can be reduced by crafting proenvironmental
arguments that resonate with the values of American conservatives” (Feinberg
and Willer 2013, 61). By paraphrasing that quotation, it is possible to summarize
the intent of this paper: Political polarization regarding environmental issues
can be avoided by appealing to widely shared American values. If the procedures
presented in this paper are followed, disagreement over pro-environmental issue frames should be minimized.
Although this paper is
about commonly shared values, many points raised here mesh well with existing
capabilities of environmental planners (Shandas and Messer 2008). One of the
best matches is that of public participation featuring a collaborative, consensus
building, problem-solving context that engages individual competence and a
sense of community pride (Pelletier and Sharp 2008), all of which are self-interest
values that have direct environmental applications. Innes and Booher (2004,
428) defined that type of consensus building/collaborative participation as an
authentic dialogue wherein all involved individuals are equally empowered and
informed. As confirmation of that fit, Burby’s (2003) study of citizen
involvement found that evidence from plan-making processes indicated that
greater stakeholder involvement resulted in stronger comprehensive plans and planning
proposals that were more likely to be implemented.
Environmental planners/managers
should also be aware that step-by-step procedural guidance in a public
participation effort is an essential form of behavior-relevant knowledge
transfer (Halford and Sheehan 1991) and can play a critical role in the
adoption of pro-environmental measures. Context-appropriate environmental information
helps to reduce individual confusion and uncertainty about how to proceed and
gives people opportunities to develop familiarity, confidence, and competence
in terms of the proposed environmental measure (De Young 1996; Kaplan 1990).
Another important fit between
applications of environmental psychology and planning involves information and education.
Many researchers believe that PEB is dependent on knowledge and learning (Gamba
and Oskamp 1994; McDaniels and Gregory 2004; Kennedy et al. 2009), which points
to another role planners/managers frequently play, that of information
dissemination. In that role it is crucial for planners to keep in mind that relationships
between environmental values/concerns/morals and behavior are stronger when
environmental knowledge is higher (Gamba and Oskamp 1994). Meinhold and Malkus (2005)
indicated that information can function as a significant moderator for the relationship
between environmental attitudes and behaviors, pointing to reasons for
strengthening the information dissemination role for environmental planners and
managers (Grant 2009; Hanna 2000).
The boundaries separating
stages on the continuum between agreement and intractable conflict may be
difficult to define with precision but are likely to include agreement –
disagreement – opposition – dispute – intractable conflict. Although some analysts
may identify fewer or more stages along that axis, the goal of this paper is for
environmental planners and managers to understand that the most critical stage
in terms of framing pro-environmental measures is the first: agreement. Every other
stage involves some level of controversy or struggle that threatens the
adoption of pro-environmental issues, policies, and programs that benefit the
public as well as the jurisdiction.
The advantages of using
a system that appeals to widely shared values are numerous and include: 1) maximizing
agreement and minimizing contention, 2) avoiding win-lose or adversarial
situations in which parties work to discredit or defeat their opponents, 3) engaging
citizens/stakeholders in competence- and consensus-building, and 4) framing
pro-environmental measures in ways that address the scope and intensity of the environmental
concerns held by most citizens and stakeholders. As environmental planners/managers
know well, the most difficult issues to resolve are not various civic actions regarding
pro-environmental policies, programs, or projects but regulatory measures that
affect real property, which is when the natural resource firm-real estate-developer
community typically springs into coordinated and well-orchestrated response to
protect their turf. One way to resolve that dilemma is for planners and
managers to organize and frame pro-environmental efforts as suggested in this
paper long before lines are drawn in the sand, opinions harden, harsh words are
exchanged, and a controversy is on the road to becoming intractable.
References
Achen,
Christopher H. 2002. Parental socialization and rational party identification. Political Behavior 24:151-170.
Ajzen, Icek, and Martin Fishbein.
1980. Understanding attitudes and
predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Alibeli,
Madalla A., and Neil R. White. 2011. The structure of environmental concern. International Journal of Business and Social
Science 2:1-8.
Allen,
James B., and Jennifer L. Ferrand. 1999. Environmental locus of control,
sympathy, and proenvironmental behavior: A test of Geller’s actively caring
hypothesis. Environment and Behavior
31:338-353.
Allmendinger, Phillip, and Mark
Tewdwr-Jones. 2002. The communicative turn in urban planning: Unravelling
paradigmatic, imperialistic and moralistic dimensions. Space and Polity 6:5-24.
Arvai, Joseph L., Robin Gregory, and Timothy L. McDaniels. 2001.
Testing a structured decision approach: Value-focused thinking for deliberative
risk communication. Risk Analysis
21:1065-1076.
Asah, Stanley T., David N. Bengston, Keith Wendt, and Kristen C. Nelson.
2012. Diagnostic reframing of intractable environmental problems: Case of a contested
multiparty public land-use conflict. Journal
of Environmental Management 108:108-119.
Bain, Paul G., Matthew J. Hornsey, Renata Bongiorno, and Carla
Jeffries. 2012. Promoting pro-environmental action in climate change deniers. Nature Climate Change 2:600-603.
Bator, Renee J., and Robert B.
Cialdini. 2000. The application of persuasion theory to the development of
effective proenvironmental public service announcements. Journal of Social Issues 56:527-541.
Batson, C. Daniel. 1994. Why act for the public good? Four answers.
Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin 20:603-610.
Booher, David E., and Judith E.
Innes. 2002. Network power in collaborative planning. Journal of Planning Education and Research 21:221-236.
Boysen, Guy A., and David L. Vogel.
2007. Biased assimilation and attitude polarization in response to learning
about biological explanations of homosexuality. Sex Roles 57:755-762.
Brandon, Gwendolyn, and Alan Lewis.
1999. Reducing household energy consumption: A qualitative and quantitative
field study. Journal of Environmental
Psychology 19:75-85.
Brody, Samuel
D. 2003a. Examining the effects of biodiversity on the ability of local plans
to manage ecological systems. Journal of
Environmental Planning and Management 46:817-837.
Brody, Samuel
D., 2003b. Measuring the effects of stakeholder participation on the quality of
local plans based on the principles of collaborative ecosystem management. Journal of Planning Education and Research
22:407-419.
Brody, Samuel
D., David R. Godschalk, and Raymond J. Burby. 2003. Mandating citizen
participation in plan making: Six strategic planning choices. Journal of the American Planning Association
69:245-264.
Brown,
Gregory G., and Patrick Reed. 2000 Validation of forest values typology for use
in national forest planning. Forest
Science 46: 240-247.
Burby, Raymond
J. 2003. Making plans that matter: Citizen involvement and government action. Journal of the American Planning Association
69:33-49.
Burgess, Heidi, and Guy Burgess.
(1996) Constructive confrontation: A transformative approach to intractable
conflicts. Conflict Resolution Quarterly
13:305–322.
Burgess,
Heidi, and Guy Burgess. 2006. Intractability and the frontier of the field. Conflict Resolution Quarterly
24:177-186.
Campbell,
Marcia C. 2003. Intractability in environmental disputes: Exploring a complex
construct. Journal of Planning Literature
17:360-371.
Campbell, Scott. (1996). Green
cities, growing cities, just cities? Urban planning and the contradictions of
sustainable development. Journal of the
American Planning Association 62:296-312.
Cantrill, James G., and Susan L.
Senecah. (2001). Using the ‘sense of self-in-place’ construct in the context of
environmental policy-making and landscape planning. Environmental Science and Policy 4:185-203.
Cialdini, Robert B. 2003. Crafting normative
measures to protect the environment. Current
Directions in Psychological Science 12:105-109.
Clary,
E. Gil, and Mark Snyder. 1999. The motivations to volunteer: Theoretical and practical
considerations. Current Directions in Psychological Science 8:156-159.
Corbett,
Julia B. 2006. Communicating nature: How
we create and understand environmental measures. Washington, D.C.: Island
Press.
Dane,
Erik, and Michael G. Pratt. 2007. Exploring intuition and its role in
managerial decision making. Academy of
Management Review 32:33-54.
Deci,
Edward L., and Richard M. Ryan. 2000. The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits:
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry 11:227-268.
de
Groot, Judith I. M., and Linda Steg. 2007. Values, beliefs, and environmental
behavior: Validation of an instrument to measure egoistic, altruistic and
biospheric value orientations in five countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 38:318-332.
de
Groot, Judith I. M., and Linda Steg. 2008. Value orientations to explain
beliefs related to environmental significant behavior: How to measure egoistic,
altruistic, and biospheric value orientations. Environment and Behavior 40:330-354.
de
Groot, Judith I. M., and Linda Steg. 2009. Mean or green: Which values can
promote stable pro-environmental behavior? Conservation
Letters 2:61-66.
De Young, Raymond. 1985. Encouraging
environmentally appropriate behavior: The role of intrinsic motivation. Journal of Environmental Systems 15:281-292.
De Young, Raymond. 1990-1991. Some psychological
aspects of living lightly: Desired lifestyle patterns and conservation behavior.
Journal of Environmental Systems 20:215-227.
De Young, Raymond. 1993. Changing
behavior and making it stick: The conceptualization and management of
conservation behavior. Environment and
Behavior 25:485-505.
De Young, Raymond. 1996. Some psychological
aspects of a reduced consumption lifestyle: The role of intrinsic satisfaction
and competence. Environment and Behavior
28:358-409.
De Young, Raymond. 2000. Expanding
and evaluating motives for environmentally responsible behavior. Journal of Social Issues 56:509-526.
Dietz, Thomas, Amy Fitzgerald, and Rachael
Shwom. 2005. Environmental values. Annual
Review of Environment and Resources 30:335-374.
Dunlap, Riley E., J. Keith Grieneeks, and
Milton Rokeach. 1983. Human values and pro-environmental behavior. In Energy and material resources: Attitudes,
values, and public policy, edited by W. David Conn, 145-169. Boulder, CO:
Westview Press.
Dunlap, Riley E., and Robert E. Jones. 2002. Environmental
concern: Conceptual and measurement issues. In Handbook of Environmental Sociology, edited by Riley Dunlap and William
Michelson, 482-542. London: Greenwood Press.
Elliott, James R., Kevin F. Gotham, and Melinda J. Mulligan. 2004.
Framing the urban: Struggles over HOPE VI and new urbanism in a historic city. City and Community 3:373-394.
Elliott, Michael, Barbara Gray, and
Roy J. Lewicki. (2002). Lessons learned about the framing and reframing of
intractable environmental conflicts. In R. J. Lewicki, B. Gray, and M. Elliott
(Eds.), Making sense of intractable
environmental conflicts: Concepts and cases (pp. 409-435). Washington, DC:
Island Press.
Elliott, Michael. (2003). Risk perception
frames in environmental decision making. Environmental
Practice, 5: 214-222.
Elliott, Michael, and Ralph Hanke.
(2003). Framing effects in toxic disputes: Cross-case analysis. In Roy Lewicki,
Barbara Gray, and Michael Elliott (Eds.). Making
sense of intractable environmental conflicts: Concepts and cases (pp.
333-351). Washington, DC: Island Press.
Elliott, Michael, and Sanda Kaufman.
(2003). Building civic capacity to resolve environmental conflicts. Environmental Practice 5:265-272.
Ernst, Robert T. (2012). The myth of
sustainable cities. Practicing Planner,
10(1). Retrieved from http://www.planning.org/practicingplanner/2012/spr/
Ernst, Robert T., and Jerry Weitz.
(2013). Moving beyond conventional environmental planning. Practicing Planner, 11, 1: Retrieved from
http://www.planning.org/practicingplanner/2013/spr/
Faludi, Andreas. (1996). Framing
with images. Environment and Planning B
23:93-108.
Faludi, Andreas. (2000). The
performance of spatial planning. Planning
Practice and Research 15:299-318.
Feinberg, Matthew, and Robb Willer.
2011. Apocalypse soon? Dire measures reduce belief in global warming by
contradicting just-world beliefs. Psychological
Science 22:34-38.
Feinberg, Matthew, and Robb Willer. 2013.
The moral roots of environmental attitudes. Psychological
Science 24:56-62.
Festinger, Leon. 1957. A theory of cognitive dissonance.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Feygina, Irina, John T. Jost, and Rachel
E. Goldsmith. 2010. System justification, the denial of global warming, and the
possibility of “system sanctioned change.” Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin 36:326-338.
Fishbein, Martin, and Icek Ajzen.
1975. Belief, attitude, intention and
behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Forester, John F. 1999. The deliberative practitioner: Encouraging
participatory planning processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Frame, Tanis M., Thomas Gunton, and J. Chad Day. 2004. The role of
collaboration in environmental management: an evaluation of land and resource
planning in British Columbia. Journal of
Environmental Planning and Management 47:59-82.
Gagnon Thompson, Suzanne C., and Michelle A. Barton. 1994.
Ecocentric and anthropocentric attitudes toward the environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology
14:149-157.
Gamba, Raymond J., and Stuart Oskamp. 1994. Factors influencing
community residential participation in commingled curbside recycling programs. Environment and Behavior 26:587-612.
Geller, E. Scott. 1995. Actively caring for the environment: An
integration of behaviorism and humanism. Environment
and Behavior 27:184-195.
Goetz, Edward G. 2008. Words matter: The importance of issue
framing and the case of affordable housing. Journal
of the American Planning Association 74:222-229.
Graham, Jesse, Jonathan Haidt, Spassena Koleva, Matt Motyl, Ravi
Iyer, Sean P. Wojcik, and Peter H. Ditto. 2013. Moral foundations theory: The
pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology 47:55-130.
Graham, Jesse, Jonathan Haidt, and Brian A. Nosek. 2009. Liberals
and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
96:1029-1046.
Graham, Jesse, Brian A. Nosek, Jonathan Haidt, Ravi Iyer, Spassena
Koleva, and Peter H. Ditto. 2011. Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
101:366-385.
Grant, Jill L. 2009. Experiential planning: A practitioner’s
account of Vancouver’s success. Journal
of the American Planning Association 75:358-370.
Gregory, Robin. 2000. Using stakeholder values to make smarter
environmental decisions. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development 42(5): 34-44.
Gregory, Robin, Timothy L. McDaniels, and Daryl Fields. 2001.
Decision aiding, not dispute resolution: Creating insights through structured
environmental decisions. Journal of
Policy Analysis and Management 20:415-432.
Gromet, Dena M., Howard Kunreuther, and Richard P. Larrick. 2013.
Political ideology affects energy-efficiency attitudes and choices. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences 110:9314-9319.
Gualini, Enrico, and Stan J. H. Majoor.
(2007). Innovative practices in large urban development projects: Conflicting
frames in the quest for ‘new urbanity’. Planning
Theory and Practice 8:297-318.
Gunton, Thomas I., and J. Chad Day 2003. The theory and practice
of collaborative planning in resource and environmental management. Environments: A Journal of Interdisciplinary
Studies 31(2): 5-19.
Haidt, Jonathan. 2008. Morality. Perspectives on Psychology Science 3:65-72.
Haidt, Jonathan, and Jesse Graham. 2007. When morality opposes
justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research 20:98-116.
Haidt, Jonathan, Jesse Graham, and Craig Joseph. 2009. Above and
below left-right: Ideological narratives and moral foundations. Psychological Inquiry: An International
Journal for the Advancement of Psychological Theory 20: 110-119.
Halford, Graeme, and Peter W. Sheehan. 1991. Human response to
environmental changes. International
Journal of Psychology 26:599-611.
Hanna, Kevin S. 2000. The paradox of participation and the hidden
role of information: A case study. Journal
of the American Planning Association 66:398-410.
Hawkins, Christopher V., and Xiao Hu Wang. 2012. Sustainable development
governance: Citizen participation and support networks in local sustainability
initiatives. Public Works Management
Policy 17:7-29.
Healey,
Patsy. 1996. The communicative turn in planning theory and its implications for
spatial strategy formations. Environment
and Planning B 23:217-234.
Healey,
Patsy. 2003. Collaborative planning in perspective. Planning Theory 2:101-123.
Healey, Patsy. 2004. The treatment
of space and place in the new strategic spatial planning in Europe. International Journal of Urban and Regional
Research 28:45-67.
Hensler,
Deborah R. 2003. Our courts, ourselves: How the alternative dispute resolution
movement is re-shaping our legal system. Pennsylvania
State Law Review 108:165-197. Accessed May 2, 2013.
http://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/reprints/2005/RP1090.pdf.
Hines, Jody
M., Harold R. Hungerford, and Audrey N. Tomera. 1986-1987. Analysis and
synthesis of research on responsible pro-environmental behaviour: A
meta-analysis. Journal of Environmental
Education, 18, 2: 1-8.
Hitlin, Steven,
and Jane Allyn Piliavin. 2004. Values: Reviving a dormant concept. Annual Review of Sociology 30:359-393.
Illsleya, David, and Tim Richardson. 2004. New national parks for
Scotland: Coalitions in conflict over the allocation of planning powers in the
Cairngorms. Journal of Environmental
Planning and Management 47(2): 219-242.
Innes, Judith E. 1995. Planning theory’s emerging paradigm: Communicative
action and interactive practice. Journal
of Planning Education and Research 14:183-191.
Innes, Judith E. 1996. Planning through consensus building: A new
view of the comprehensive planning ideal. Journal
of American Planning Association 62:460-72.
Innes, Judith E. 2004. Consensus building: Clarifications for the
critics. Planning Theory 3:5-20.
Innes, Judith E., and David E. Booher. 2004. Reframing public
participation: Strategies for the 21st Century. Planning Theory and Practice 5:419-436.
Jensen, Michael C. 1994. Self-interest, altruism, incentives, and
agency theory. Journal of Applied
Corporate Finance 7(2): 40-45.
Jensen, Ole B., and Tim Richardson.
(2003). Being on the map: The new iconographies of power over European space. International Planning Studies 8:9-34.
Kahan, Dan M. 2013. Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive
reflection: An experimental study. Judgment
and Decision Making 8:407-424.
Kahan, Dan M., Maggie M. Wittlin, Ellen Peters, Paul Slovic, Lisa
Larrimore Ouellette, Donald Braman, and Gregory N. Mandel, 2012. The polarizing
impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks. Nature Climate Change 2:732-735.
Kahn, Matthew E., and Eric A. Morris. 2009. Walking the walk: The
association between community environmentalism and green travel behavior. Journal of the American Planning Association
75, 389-405.
Kaplan, Stephen. 1990. Being needed, adaptive muddling and
human-environment relationships. In Coming
of age: Proceedings of the
Environmental Design Research Association Conference, edited by R. I. Selby, Kathryn H. Anthony, Jean Choi, and
Brian Orland, 19-25. Oklahoma City, OK: Environmental Design Research
Association. Accessed June 26, 2013. http://www.edra.org/sites/default/files/publications/EDRA21-Kaplan-19-25.pdf.
Kaplan, Stephen. 2000. Human nature and environmentally
responsible behavior. Journal of Social
Issues 56:491-508.
Karl, Herman A., Lawrence E. Susskind, and Katherine H. Wallace. 2007.
A dialogue, not a diatribe: Effective integration of science and policy through
joint fact finding. Environment: Science
and Policy for Sustainable Development 49:20-34.
Karp, David
G. 1996. Values and their effects on pro-environmental behavior. Environment and Behavior 28:111-133.
Kaufman, Sanda, Robert Gardner, and Guy
Burgess. 2003. Just the facts, please: Framing and technical information. Environmental Practice 5:223-231.
Kaufman, Sanda, and Barbara Gray. 2003. Using retrospective and
prospective frame elicitation to evaluate environmental disputes. In The promise and performance of environmental
conflict resolution, edited by Lisa B. Bingham and Rosemary O’Leary, 129-147. Washington, DC: Resources for
the Future.
Kaufman, Sanda, and Janet Smith. (1999).
Framing and reframing in land use change conflicts. Journal of Architectural and Planning Research 16:164-180.
Kennedy, Emily Huddart, Thomas M. Beckley, Bonita L. McFarlane, and
Solange Nadeau, 2009. Why we don’t “walk the talk”: Understanding the
environmental values/behaviour gap in Canada. Human Ecology Review 16:151-160.
Kolb, Deborah M., and Jeffrey Z. Rubin, 1991. Mediation from a
disciplinary perspective. In Handbook of
negotiation research. Vol. 3, Research
on negotiation in organizations, edited by Max H. Bazerman, Roy J. Lewicki,
and Blair H. Sheppard. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Kollmuss,
Anja, and Julian Agyeman. 2002. Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally
and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research 8:239-260.
Kurz,
Tim. 2002. The psychology of environmentally sustainable behavior: Fitting
together pieces of the puzzle. Analyses
of Social Issues and Public Policy 2:257-278.
Lewicki,
Roy J., Stephen E. Weiss, and David Lewin. 1992. Models of conflict,
negotiation and third party intervention: A review and synthesis. Journal of Organizational Behavior 13:209-252.
Lewicki,
Roy J., Barbara Gray, and Michael Elliott, eds. 2003. Making sense of intractable environmental conflicts: Concepts and cases.
Washington, DC: Island Press.
Lewis,
Paul G., and Mark Baldassare. 2010. The complexity of public attitudes toward
compact development: Survey evidence from five states. Journal of the American Planning Association 76:219-237.
Lindenberg,
Siegwart, and Linda Steg. 2007. Normative, gain and hedonic goal frames guiding
environmental behavior. Journal of Social
Issues 63:117-137.
Lindseth, Gard. (2004). The Cities
for Climate Protection Campaign (CCPC) and the framing of local climate policy.
Local Environment 9:325-336.
Loroz,
Peggy S. 2007. The interaction of message frames and reference points in
prosocial persuasive appeals.
Psychology and Marketing 24:1001-1023.
Majoor, Stan J. H. (2011). Framing
large-scale projects: Barcelona forum and the challenge of balancing local and
global needs. Journal of Planning
Education and Research 31:143-156.
Malczewski, Jacek, Micja Pazner, and
Malgodzata Zaliwska. (1997). GIS-based techniques for visualizing multicriteria
location analysis: a case study. Cartography
and Geographic Information Systems 24:80-90.
Mansbridge,
Jane J. 1990. Beyond self-interest.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Markowitz, Ezra M., Lewis R. Goldberg, Michael C. Ashton, and Kibeom
Lee. 2012. Profiling the “pro-environmental individual”: A personality
perspective. Journal of Personality
80:81-111.
McAdams,
Dan, Michelle Albaugh, Emily Farber, Jennifer Daniels, Regina Logan, and Brad
Olson, 2008. Family metaphors and moral intuitions: How conservatives and
liberals narrate their lives. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 95:978-990.
McCann, Eugene J. (2003). Framing
space and time in the city: Urban policy and the politics of spatial and
temporal scale. Journal of Urban Affairs,
25: 159-178.
McCright, Aaron M., and Riley E. Dunlap. 2011. The politicization
of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global
warming, 2001-2010. The Sociological
Quarterly 52:155-194.
McEvoy, Darryn, Hartmut Fünfgeld, and
Karyn Bosomworth. 2013. Resilience and climate change adaptation: The
importance of framing. Planning Practice
and Research 28:280-293.
McDaniels,
Timothy L., and Robin Gregory. 2004. Learning as an objective within a
structured risk management decision process. Environmental Science and Technology 38:1921-1926.
McKenzie-Mohr,
Doug. 2000a. Promoting sustainable behavior: An introduction to community-based
social marketing. Journal of Social
Issues 56:543-554.
McKenzie-Mohr,
Doug. 2000b. Fostering sustainable behavior through community-based social
marketing. American Psychologist
55:531-537.
Meinhold,
Jana L., and Amy J. Malkus. 2005. Adolescent environmental behaviors: Can
knowledge, attitudes, and self-efficacy make a difference? Environment and Behavior 37:511-532.
Merrick,
Jason R. W., and Margot W. Garcia. 2004. Using value-focused thinking to
improve watersheds. Journal of the
American Planning Association 70:313-327.
Moisander, Johanna. 2007.
Motivational complexity of green consumerism. International Journal of Consumer Studies 31:404-409.
Norton, Bryan G., and Anne C.
Steinemann. 2001 Environmental values and adaptive management. Environmental Values 10:473-506.
Omoto, Allen M., and Mark Snyder.
1995. Sustained helping without obligation: Motivation, longevity of service,
and perceived attitude change among AIDS volunteers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68:671-686.
O’Riordan,
Timothy. 2005. Inclusive and community participation in the coastal zone:
opportunities and dangers. In Managing
European coasts: Past, present and future, edited by Jan E. Vermaat, Wim
Salomons, Laurens Bouwer, and Kerry Turner, 173-184. Berlin: Springer.
Pelletier,
Luc G., and Elizabeth Sharp. 2008. Persuasive communication and
proenvironmental behaviours: How message tailoring and message framing can
improve the integration of behaviours through self-determined motivation. Canadian Psychology 49:210-217.
Poortinga, Wouter, Linda Steg, and Charles
Vlek. 2004. Values, environmental concern, and environmental behavior: A study
into household energy use. Environment
and Behavior 36:70-93.
Pruitt,
Dean G. 1995. Process and outcome in community mediation. Negotiation Journal 11:365-377.
Pruitt,
Dean G., Robert S. Pierce, Josephine M. Zubek, Neal B. McGillicuddy, and Gary L.
Welton. 1993. Determinants of short-term and long-term success in mediation. In
Conflict between people and groups:
Causes, processes, and resolutions, edited by Stephen Worchel and Jeffrey
A. Simpson, 60-73. Chicago: Nelson Hall, Inc.
Reed,
Patrick, and Gregory G. Brown. 2003. Values suitability analysis: A methodology
for identifying and integrating public perceptions of ecosystem values in
forest planning. Journal of Environmental
Planning and Management 46: 643-658.
Renn, Ortwin. 1999. A model for an
analytic-deliberate process in risk management. Environmental Science and Technology 33:3049-3055
Rokeach, Milton. 1968. Beliefs, attitudes and values: A theory of
organization and change. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Rokeach, Milton. 1971. Long-range
experimental modification of values, attitudes, and behavior. American Psychologist 26:453-459.
Rokeach, Milton. 1973. The nature of human values. New York:
John Wiley.
Rokeach, Milton, and Sandra J. Ball-Rokeach.
1989. Stability and change in American value priorities, 1968-1981. American Psychologist 44:775-784.
Ryan, Richard M., and Edward L. Deci.
2000. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
social development, and well-being. American
Psychologist 55:68-78.
Salet, Willem. (2008). Rethinking
urban projects: Experiences in Europe. Urban
Studies 45:2343-2363.
Schon, Donald A., and Martin Rein.
1995. Frame reflection: Toward the
resolution of intractable policy controversies. New York: Basic Books.
Schultz, P. Wesley. 2002. Environmental
attitudes and behaviors across cultures. Online
Readings in Psychology and Culture 8(1). Accessed June 12, 2013. http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol8/iss1/4/.
Schultz, P. Wesley, and Lynnette Zelezny.
2003. Reframing environmental measures to be congruent with American values. Human Ecology Review 10:126-136.
Schwartz, Shalom H. 1992. Universals
in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical
tests in 20 countries. In Advances in
experimental social psychology, vol. 25, edited by Mark P. Zanna, 1-65. New York: Academic Press.
Schwartz, Shalom H. 1994a. Beyond
individualism/collectivism: New cultural dimensions of values. In Individualism and collectivism: Theory,
method, and applications; Cross-cultural
research and methodology series, vol. 18 edited by Uichol Kim, Harry C.
Triandis, Çigdem Kâğitçibaşi, Sang-Chin Choi, and Gene Yoon, 85-119. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994b). Are there
universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? Journal of
Social Issues 50: 19-45.
Schwartz, Shalom H., and Wolfgang Bilsky.
1987. Toward a universal psychological structure of human values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
53:550-562.
Shandas,
Vivek, and W. Barry Messer. 2008. “Fostering green communities through civic
engagement: Community-based environmental stewardship in the Portland area.” Journal of the American Planning Association
74:408-418.
Shmueli,
Deborah F. 2008. Framing in geographical analysis of environmental conflicts:
Theory, methodology and three case studies. Geoforum
39:2048-2061. Accessed June 21, 2013. http://132.68.226.240/english/pdf/Professors/Devorah_Shmueli/9.pdf.
Shmueli, Deborah, Michael Elliott, and
Sanda Kaufman. (2006). Frame changes and the management of intractable
conflicts. Conflict Resolution Quarterly
24:207-218.
Shove,
Elizabeth. 2010. Beyond the ABC: Climate change policy and theories of social
change. Environment and Planning A 42:1273-1285.
Snyder,
Mark, and Allen M. Omoto. 1991. Who helps and why: The psychology of AIDS volunteerism.
In Helping and being helped: Naturalistic
studies, edited by Shirlynn Spacapan and Stuart Oskamp, 213-239. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Steg,
Linda, Lieke Dreijerink, and Wokje Abrahamse. 2005. Factors influencing the
acceptability of energy policies: A test of VBN theory. Journal of Environmental Psychology 25:415-425.
Steg,
Linda, Judith I. M. de Groot, Lieke Dreijerink, Wokje Abrahamse, and Frans Siero.
2011. General antecedents of personal norms, policy acceptability, and
intentions: The role of values, worldviews, and environmental concern. Society and Natural Resources: An
International Journal 24:349-367.
Stern, Paul C. 2000. Toward a coherent
theory of environmentally significant behavior. Journal of Social Issues 56:407-424.
Stern, Paul C., and Thomas Dietz.
1994. The value basis of environmental concern. Journal of Social Issues 50:65-84.
Stern,
Paul C., Thomas Dietz, Troy Abel, Gregory A. Guagnano, and Linda Kalof. 1999. A
value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of
environmental concern. Human Ecology
Review 6:81-97.
Stern, Paul C., Thomas Dietz, and
Linda Kalof. 1993. Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern. Environment and Behavior 25:322-348.
Stern, Paul C., Linda Kalof, Thomas
Dietz, and Gregory A. Guagnano. 1995. Values, beliefs, and proenvironmental
action: Attitude formation toward emergent attitude objects. Journal of Applied Social Psychology
25:1611-1636.
Struch, Naomi, and Shalom H.
Schwartz. 1989. Intergroup aggression: Its predictors and distinctness from
in-group bias. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 56:364-373.
Susskind,
Lawrence E. 2009. Deliberative democracy and dispute resolution. Ohio State Journal on Dispute Resolution
24:395-406. Accessed July 1, 2013. http://cbuilding.org/sites/cbuilding.org/files/OHIO_JDR_Deliberative.Democracy.pdf.
Susskind,
Lawrence E., and Herman A. Karl. 2009. Balancing science and politics in
environmental decision-making: A new role for science impact coordinators. Science Impact Collaborative Paper. Accessed
July 1, 2013. http://www.academia.edu/2801306/Balancing_Science_and_Politics_in_Environmental_Decision-Making_A_New_Role_for_Science_Impact_Coordinators.
Susskind,
Lawrence E., and Jeffrey L. Cruikshank. 2006. Breaking Robert’s rules: The new way to run your meeting, build
consensus, and get results. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Susskind,
Lawrence E., and Alan Weinstein. 1980. Towards a theory of environmental
dispute resolution. Boston College
Environmental Affairs Law Review 9:311-357.
Susskind, Lawrence E., Mieke van der
Wansem, and Armand Ciccarelli. 2000. Mediating
land use disputes: Pros and cons. Cambridge, MA: Lincoln Institute of Land
Policy.
Taber, Charles S., and Milton Lodge.
2006. Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science
50:755-769.
Taylor, Dorcetta E. (2000). The rise
of the environmental justice paradigm: Injustice framing and the social construction
of environmental discourses. American
Behavioral Scientist 43:508-580.
Triandis,
Harry C. 1996. The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist 51:407-415.
Turner,
Ralph H. 1991. The use and misuse of rational models in collective behavior and
social psychology. European Journal of
Sociology 32:84-108.
Tversky,
Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1981. The framing of decisions and the rationality
of choice. Science 221:453-458.
Weber, Elke U., and Paul C. Stern.
2011. Public understanding of climate change in the United States. American Psychologist 66:315-328.
Wondolleck, Julia M., and Steven L.
Yaffee. 2000. Making collaboration work:
Lessons from innovations in natural resource management. Washington, DC:
Island Press.
Endnotes
[1] Also known in the literature as
Environmentally Responsible Behavior and Environmentally Sustainable Behavior.
[2] The term indicates decision-making
agents in situations characterized by emotions, various types of uncertainty,
and time and knowledge constraints. Non-rational behavior must not be confused
with irrational behavior.
[3] Which De Young termed
Environmentally Responsible Behavior or ERB.
[4] Following norms and established
processes is also a self-interest orientation.
[5] Empirical research by Alibeli and
White (2011) and de Groot and Steg (2008) demonstrated that the three
value-based concerns are independent of each other and distinctions among them
are statistically reliable and valid.
[6] See http://wilderness.org/why
[7] See http://www.sierraclub.org/
[8] See
http://www.sierraclub.org/ej/default.aspx
[9] See http://www.nwf.org/
[10] See
http://worldwildlife.org/initiatives
[11] See http://www.edf.org/what-we-do
[12] See http://www.audubon.org/about-us
[13] See
http://worldwildlife.org/species
No comments:
Post a Comment